Considering Context

by

One of the central premises of social psychology is the power of the situation. The very definition of social psychology reflects this, pointing out the influence of others on thoughts, feelings, and actions.  In this discussion, we will consider contextualization by evaluating the fundamental and far-reaching role of culture.
To inform your thinking on this topic, begin by reading “Toward a Psychological Science for a Cultural Species” (Heine & Norenzayan, 2006), “Lessons Learned from a Lifetime of Applied Social Psychological Research” (Ross, 2004), and “Reflections on the Stanford Prison Experiment: Genesis, Rransformations, Consequences” (Zimbardo, Maslach, & Haney, 2000).
Then, locate a peer-reviewed empirical article (see Scholarly, Peer Reviewed, and Other Credible Sources (Links to an external site.)) describing a research study that examines a psychological phenomenon from a cultural perspective.  Discuss the research, considering the various elements of a critical review (see Using a Scientific Journal Article to Write a Critical Review (Links to an external site.)) with reference to/explanation of the more broad social-pychological domain (social thinking, social relations, social influence).  Appraise the role of culture in our psychological understanding of this phenomenon.  Assess the relevance of one “lesson” of applied psychology (Ross, 2004) to your selected study.

Toward a Psychological Science
for a Cultural Species
Steven J. Heine and Ara Norenzayan

Never use plagiarized sources. Get Your Original Essay on
Considering Context
Hire Professionals Just from $11/Page
Order Now Click here

University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

ABSTRACT—Humans are a cultural species, and the study

of human psychology benefits from attention to cultural

influences. Cultural psychology’s contributions to psycho-

logical science can largely be divided according to the two

different stages of scientific inquiry. Stage 1 research seeks

cultural differences and establishes the boundaries of

psychological phenomena. Stage 2 research seeks under-

lying mechanisms of those cultural differences. The liter-

atures regarding these two distinct stages are reviewed,

and various methods for conducting Stage 2 research are

discussed. The implications of culture-blind and multi-

cultural psychologies for society and intergroup relations

are also discussed.

Before we inquire into origins and functional relations, it is nec-

essary to know the thing we are trying to explain. (Asch, 1952/

1987, p. 65)

Humans are a cultural species. Cultural learning, or the ability

to acquire behaviors from other individuals, is evident in a va-

riety of different species (Lefebvre & Giraldeau, 1994; Rendell

&Whitehead, 2001; Whiten et al., 1999). However, humans are

unique in the extent to which cultural learning accumulates

rapidly over generations, radically alters the ecology in which

humans live, and pervades their full repertoire of thoughts and

behaviors (Richerson & Boyd, 2005; Tomasello, 1999). There-

fore, a rich understanding of how humans’ minds operate would

be facilitated by a psychological science that is able to study

how specific cultural experiences shape and express universal

biological potentials.

Few people would dispute that culture is relevant to psy-

chology. Yet for much of the history of their field, most psy-

chologists have sought to discover and explain human thought

and behavior in terms of universal principles that are applicable

to all people (at least all non-brain-damaged adults without

clinical disorders) regardless of the cultural and historical

contexts in which their minds develop and operate. Granted,

assumptions of universality are sometimes empirically exam-

ined in developmental research and in gender comparisons that

attempt to disentangle effects of innate structures and matura-

tion from those of experience or socialization. But it has been the

primary goal of cultural psychology to transform this assumption

of universality into an empirically testable hypothesis. Cultural

psychology, and cross-cultural comparisons more broadly, has

enjoyed tremendous growth over the past two decades, and has

moved from themargins of psychology to the central theories and

findings of the field.

Clearly, there are many possible ways one can approach a

project as ambitious and complex as the study of how psycho-

logical patterns are manifested across cultures, and even within

cultural psychology there are different views and lively debates.

Here we offer our own view of the field. The centrality of cross-

cultural comparisons to progress in all areas of psychology is

discussed, and recent advances in cross-cultural research are

outlined.We consider some outstanding issues and critiques and

recommend avenues for future research. A central theme in our

article is that cultural psychology, as is the case with any other

scientific field, advances in two overlapping but distinct stages

of inquiry. Each stage has its own logic and priorities, an issue to

which we now turn.

TWO STAGES OF SCIENTIFIC INQUIRY

Most scientific inquiry proceeds through two stages. In the first

stage, new theories that facilitate the observation and discovery of

interesting phenomena are proposed, and various methodological

confounds are ruled out. In the second stage, the inner workings of

phenomena are more precisely explained, and underlying mech-

anisms are identified. The first stage continues while the second is

under way, because scientific explanations critically depend on

the expansion of the database to novel domains and the discovery

of additional phenomena. Philosophers and historians of science,

although disagreeing about the extent of the role of a priori theories

in guiding observation, say that most sciences seem to mature in

Address correspondence to Steven J. Heine or Ara Norenzayan,
Department of Psychology, 2136 West Mall, University of British
Columbia, Vancouver, BC, V6T 1Z4 Canada, e-mail: [email protected]
ubc.ca or [email protected]

PERSPECTIVES ON PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

Volume 1—Number 3 251Copyright r 2006 Association for Psychological Science

this sequence (Hempel, 1965; Salmon, 1984). Consider Darwin’s

theory of evolution by natural selection. Darwin developed his

theory of natural selection and embarked on decades of systematic

cross-species observations and meticulous documentation of fea-

tures of various organisms and the ecological peculiarities of their

habitats (Darwin, 1859/1958). His theory hinged critically on the

idea that biological properties are inherited, yet themechanisms of

inheritance were unknown at the time, and his proposal for how

inheritance worked was ultimately flawed. One of Darwin’s influ-

ential critics, Fleeming Jenkin, correctly pointed out that if in-

heritance were the result of taking the average of the features from

the parental contributions, as Darwin proposed, then in each

generational transmission, variation would be cut in half. In a few

generations, there would be little variation left for inheritance, and

natural selection would falter (Boyd & Silk, 2003). Only decades

later was it understood that parental genes remain discrete entities

in reproduction, and Darwin’s theory became grounded in the

principles of Mendelian genetics, which then ushered in the

modern synthesis between evolutionary theory and genetics,

forming the basis of modern biology (Dobzhansky, 1962).

Much of psychology also operates this way (for discussions, see

Cronbach, 1986, and Rozin, 2001). Theories are proposed and

revised in the first-stage process of predicting and discovering

interesting phenomena, but the precise mechanisms underlying

theoretical claims are often poorly understood prior to the second

stage of investigation. We agree with Rozin (2001) that Stage 1

research is a key element of scientific progress in any growing

science, and that unnecessary constraints on this type of research

can damage the prospects of a discipline to develop into a mature

science. Asmore andmore interesting phenomena are discovered,

Stage 2 research is initiated; scientists begin to offer competing

explanations for them, presumed mechanisms eventually become

the subject of debates, and competing accounts for mechanisms

are advanced, animating scientific discovery for long periods of

time. These debates, in turn, may lead to additional discoveries

of interesting phenomena. Only with time and patience are

controversies resolved successfully; paradigms sometimes shift,

and scientific consensus regarding mechanisms emerges.

Research in cultural psychology can also be broken down into

these two stages (see Atran, Medin, & Ross, 2005, for a similar

observation). Stage 1 research typically proposes theories that

predict cultural differences in particular psychological processes,

whereas Stage 2 research typically seeks tomore precisely explain

the observed cultural differences by identifying the critical vari-

ables that account for them. In the following sections, we articulate

how this distinction is useful for understanding how cultural

psychology can contribute to psychological science in general.

STAGE 1 CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH:
IDENTIFYING CULTURAL DIFFERENCES

The most straightforward goal of Stage 1 cultural psychological

research has been to investigate the extent to which people from

different cultures vary in psychological processes. Thus far, the

majority of cultural psychological research has been conducted

at this stage. Such research is of critical importance to scientific

progress in the field, as we explain next.

Stage 1 Research and External Validity

Psychology has long been criticized for ignoring issues of gen-

eralizability of findings, the most prominent criticism being that

its restricted database may limit the external validity of its

findings (e.g., Gergen, 1973; Medin &Atran, 2004; Rozin, 2001;

Sears, 1986). Social psychology has been especially vulnerable

to such criticisms; it investigates questions regarding how

people perceive, understand, and respond to the (culturally

variable) social world, yet most social psychological research

has been conducted within the social environment of middle-

class college students. However, nowhere are the limits of the

restricted psychological database more problematic than when

it comes to cultural representation. A recent review found that

92% of studies published in the Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, the most influential international journal in

these fields, were conducted in North America, and a full 99% of

the studies were conducted in theWest (Quinones-Vidal, Lopez-

Garcia, Penaranda-Ortega, & Tortosa-Gil, 2004). This problem

would not be severe if the publications in such journals did not

make the implicit or explicit claim that their findings are ap-

plicable broadly to humanity at large, rather than to the Western

subpopulations from which participants are selected. That a

similar inattention to cultural variability has been found in other

areas of psychology as well (Norenzayan & Heine, 2005) un-

derscores how much of the psychological database renders re-

searchers ill prepared to speak confidently of the extent to which

many psychological processes are universal.

In many research programs, there are trade-offs between

maximizing internal validity and maximizing external validity.

The tendency for researchers from a number of psychological

disciplines to primarily conduct studies with convenience

samples of Western college students, who are disproportionately

of European descent and of middle-class background, suggests

that psychologists are often inclined to privilege the mainte-

nance of internal validity. That is, studies conducted with con-

venience samples afford opportunities for researchers to more

easily conduct rigorous and systematic series of studies that can

address competing hypotheses and rule out methodological

artifacts. Although a heavy reliance on convenience samples

facilitates the construction of sound theories that are capable of

making reliable predictions, the incumbent sacrifice of external

validity becomes untenable when researchers raise questions

regarding human nature. The limited psychological database

raises a cloud of doubt regarding the generalizability of many

findings. How can researchers know whether they are study-

ing a phenomenon that is characteristic of humans everywhere

or whether they have identified a cultural product that

emerges from participating in Western middle-class culture?

252 Volume 1—Number 3

Psychological Science for a Cultural Species

This distinction cannot confidently be made until appropriate

cross-cultural research tactics are used to test whether the

phenomenon meets the criteria of various levels of universality

(for a review, see Norenzayan & Heine, 2005).

The question of the generalizability of research findings is a

challenge for science more generally, and perhaps ensuring

culturally representative sampling is an onerous technicality

that is unnecessary for psychology. For example, it would hardly

seem reasonable to say that a reliably observed phenomenon

such as social facilitation or transfer-appropriate processing

might not be a universal feature of humankind because no one

has ever investigated it among, say, the Trobriand Islanders.

Such a claim would be dubious because there is no theoretical

basis for anticipating any differences in social facilitation or

transfer-appropriate processing in this population. Were it the

case, however, that one did have compelling a priori theoretical

reasons to anticipate that either of these processes would be

different among the Trobriand Islanders, psychologists’ under-

standing of that process would potentially be in need of revision

if it were indeed found that this population performed reliably

differently on relevant tasks.

Stage 1 Research Identifies Cultural Variation in

Psychological Processes

Recent theoretical and empirical developments in cultural

psychology have brought the field to the point where researchers

need to be mindful of the generalizability of North American, or

more generally Western, findings to other cultural contexts.

There are a number of rich theoretical models that allow for

predictions about the extent to which various models will rep-

licate in other cultural contexts (e.g., D. Cohen & Hoshino-

Browne, 2005; Heine, 2001; Kim, 2002; Markus & Kitayama,

1991; Medin & Atran, 2004; Nisbett & Cohen, 1996; Nisbett,

Peng, Choi, &Norenzayan, 2001; Shweder,Much,Mahapatra, &

Park, 1997; Triandis, 1989). Furthermore, in the past 20 years,

since the field of cultural psychology reemerged as a significant

discipline, the great extent of documented cultural variation in

psychological processes has been rather unexpected, even for

cultural psychologists. For example, pronounced and theoreti-

cally meaningful cultural differences have been found in fun-

damental psychological processes, such as eye movements for

scanning inanimate scenes (e.g., Chua, Boland, & Nisbett,

2005); attention (Miyamoto, Nisbett, & Masuda, 2006); per-

ception of color, space, and time (e.g., Boroditsky, 2001; Lev-

inson, 1997; Roberson, Davies, & Davidoff, 2000); numerical

reasoning (e.g., Gordon, 2004); unspoken thinking (e.g., Kim,

2002); preferences for high subjective well-being (SWB; e.g.,

Diener, Diener, & Diener, 1995); the manifestation of psycho-

logical disorders such as depression (e.g., Kleinman, 1982;

Ryder et al., 2005) and bulimia nervosa (e.g., Keel & Klump,

2003); the need for high self-esteem (e.g., Heine, Lehman,

Markus, & Kitayama, 1999); and preferences for formal rea-

soning (e.g., Norenzayan, Smith, Kim, & Nisbett, 2002). Re-

search programs in these areas have demonstrated that culture

is implicated at a much more fundamental level of psychologi-

cal processing than what was previously considered, and these

findings are forcing researchers to conceive of these phenomena

differently than they had before.

Predicted cultural differences have also emerged in a diverse

array of phenomena, including how people handle contradic-

tion (e.g., Peng & Nisbett, 1999), prevention and promotion

orientations (e.g., Lee, Aaker, & Gardner, 2000), self-concepts

(e.g., Cousins, 1989), moral intuitions and reasoning (A.B. Co-

hen & Rozin, 2001; Miller & Bersoff, 1992), tendencies to make

situational and dispositional attributions (e.g., Choi, Nisbett, &

Norenzayan, 1999; Morris & Peng, 1994), preferences for

choices made by oneself or by others (e.g., Iyengar & Lepper,

1999), the nature of friendships and enemyships (e.g., Adams,

2005), cognitive dissonance (e.g., Heine & Lehman, 1997b;

Kitayama, Snibbe, Markus, & Suzuki, 2004), memories for focal

and background objects (e.g., Masuda & Nisbett, 2001), moti-

vations for uniqueness (e.g., Kim & Markus, 1999), certain

kinds of category-based inductive reasoning (e.g., Medin, Ross,

Atran, Burnett, & Block, 2002), daily variability in affective

experiences (Oishi, Diener, Napa Scollon, & Biswas-Diener,

2004), the importance of romantic love in marriage decisions (R.

Levine, Sato, Hashimoto, & Verma, 1995), the pace of life and

time perspective (R.V. Levine & Norenzayan, 1999), preferred

decisions in the ultimatum game (e.g., Henrich et al., 2005),

feelings of control (e.g., Morling, Kitayama, &Miyamoto, 2002),

and predilection for violence in response to insults (e.g., Nisbett

& Cohen, 1996), to name several. Systematic cultural differ-

ences have also been found in early childhood (e.g., Grossmann,

Grossmann, Spangler, Suess, & Unzner, 1985; Imai & Gentner,

1997; Tardif, 1996), underscoring just how embedded psycho-

logical life is within cultural experiences.

Furthermore, many of these cultural differences are pro-

nounced in magnitude. Meta-analyses reveal large effects (av-

erage d > .80) for the difference in the magnitude of self-

enhancement motivation between East Asians and Westerners

(Heine & Hamamura, 2006), moderate to large effects for cog-

nitive differences between East Asians andWesterners (average

d 5 .60; Miyamoto, Kitayama, & Talhelm, 2006), and smaller

effects for cultural differences in self-report measures of indi-

vidualism and collectivisms between Asians and Americans

(average ds 5 .39 for individualism and .24 for collectivism;

Matsumoto, 2006; Oyserman, Coon, & Kemmelmeier, 2002). In

general, the cultural differences tend to be more pronounced in

studies that compare behaviors that reflect implicit psycholog-

ical tendencies and less pronounced in studies that compare

explicit self-reported cultural values on subjective Likert scales

(for discussion, see Heine, Lehman, Peng, & Greenholtz, 2002;

Kitayama, 2002).

Stage 1 cultural psychological research is an ongoing project

as more and more theories are developed and psychological

Volume 1—Number 3 253

Steven J. Heine and Ara Norenzayan

phenomena are tested in a growing array of cultures. As it

progresses, this enterprise will paint an increasingly detailed

picture regarding the kinds of processes that are most affected

by cultural influences, and those that operate the most inde-

pendently of cultural context. It is difficult to know a priori

which psychological processes are most susceptible to cultural

variation, so there is no alternative to solid, programmatic Stage

1 research. For example, growing research indicates that at-

tentional processes, initially believed to be fixed and universal,

are in some respects highly responsive to cultural experience

(e.g., Chua et al., 2005). Conversely, some core aspects of rea-

soning about other peoples’ mental states (or theory of mind ),

initially believed to be a folk theory that is culturally malleable

to a large degree, appear to develop in remarkably similar ways

across cultures (e.g., Callaghan et al., 2005).

One important component of Stage 1 research has been to

identify the specific situations in which some cultural differ-

ences in psychological processes are made manifest. Cultural

differences in various processes are often fluid and do not

emerge uniformly as main effects; they often are evident only

when certain contextual variables are present. For example,

people participating in cultures of honor are not more aggressive

than other people across all situations; rather, their aggression

emerges specifically in situations in which their honor has been

slighted (Nisbett & Cohen, 1996); the Protestant work ethic does

not encourage an overall more detached attitude toward rela-

tionships, and cultural differences emerge only when Protes-

tants and non-Protestants are engaged in a work task (e.g.,

Sanchez-Burks, 2002); East Asians do not always prefer intui-

tive reasoning strategies more than Westerners, but show the

same preferences and skills for formal reasoning when com-

pleting abstract tasks (e.g., Norenzayan et al., 2002). In sum,

much Stage 1 cultural psychological research has targeted the

sensitivity of various psychological processes to situational

variables and investigated how cultural differences often appear

only in specific contexts.

A second key focus of Stage 1 research has been to conduct

systematic series of studies to rule out competing artifactual

accounts of cultural differences. A particular challenge of

conducting psychological research across cultures is that there

are many unique methodological concerns that arise regarding

the interpretability of findings (for reviews, see D. Cohen, in

press; Greenfield, 1997; and van de Vijver & Leung, 2000). The

wide array of methodological artifacts that are of concern to

cultural psychologists has resulted in research on cross-cultural

comparability developing into an enterprise in and of itself.

Efforts to determine the validity of cultural differences consti-

tute a large part of the studies that are conducted in Stage 1

cultural psychological research.

In sum, the range of identified cultural differences in psy-

chological phenomena has expanded significantly in the past 20

years. Many of these findings have emerged in the wake of recent

theoretical developments in cultural psychology, most notably

the distinction between independent and interdependent con-

struals of self (Markus & Kitayama, 1991) and individualism-

collectivism (Triandis, 1989). These theoretical developments,

coupled with the burgeoning database of documented cultural

variation in fundamental psychological processes, challenge

psychologists to be hesitant in assuming that findings that

emerge from a single population must necessarily be psycho-

logical universals (Norenzayan & Heine, 2005).

Stage 1 Cross-Cultural Research Informs Theories About

Universals

Stage 1 cultural psychological research is as important in doc-

umenting robust similarities across cultures as it is in docu-

menting variability. Such research allows psychologists to

identify the extent to which psychological phenomena are cul-

ture-specific or are psychological universals (Norenzayan &

Heine, 2005; Norenzayan, Schaller, & Heine, in press). We

submit that the restricted database that has historically char-

acterized psychological research has led researchers to inherit a

sense of ‘‘culture-blindness’’ whereby observed findings in one’s

own culture are assumed to be universal. Furthermore, because

some degree of universality is often a central assumption of

evolutionary explanations, this culture-blindness is of particular

relevance to evolutionary explanations, and greatly complicates

efforts to articulate how particular psychological phenomena

may have evolved.

For example, consider the question of whether a need for

positive self-regard is a psychological universal. One way to

approach this question is to conceive of positive self-regard as

self-enhancement, which is usually operationalized as tenden-

cies to dwell on and elaborate positive information about the self

relative to negative information (Heine, 2005b). With this oper-

ationalization, it is clear that people from Western cultures are

motivated to have positive self-regard, as across dozens of dif-

ferent methods, Westerners show consistent and pronounced

self-enhancement (average d 5 .86). In contrast, the same

methods have revealed an average d of �.02 for East Asians
(Heine & Hamamura, 2006). Thus, a need for positive self-

regard operationalized as self-enhancement appears to be far

weaker, if not largely absent, among people participating in East

Asian cultural contexts (see Heine, Kitayama, & Hamamura, in

press, and Sedikides, Gaertner, & Vevea, 2005, for some dis-

agreement about the evidence regarding this conclusion).

Given these findings, howmight one consider the evolutionary

origins of motivations for positive self-regard? On the basis of

the consistent evidence for self-enhancement among Western-

ers, a variety of evolutionary accounts have been offered for the

emergence of this motivation. For example, the self-enhance-

ment motive has been posited to have been selected (a) as a

gauge of changes in one’s status within dominance hierarchies

(Barkow, 1989), (b) as a barometer of the vulnerability of one’s

social relationships (Leary, Tambor, Terdal, & Downs, 1995),

254 Volume 1—Number 3

Psychological Science for a Cultural Species

or (c) to stave off the debilitating effects of existential anxie-

ties arising from the awareness of one’s impending death

(Pyszczynski, Greenberg, & Solomon, 2004). However, these

theories are rendered less compelling when one considers the

relative lack of self-enhancement motivations that is evident

among East Asians, especially as it seems that concerns with

status, social relationships, and death are at least as strong in

East Asia as they are in the West (e.g., Heine, 2001; Heine,

Harihara, & Niiya, 2002).

We suggest that compelling evolutionary accounts for the or-

igins of psychological processes need to consider the adaptive

value of the processes at a level of abstraction where universality

is more evident, or they need to specify the conditions under

which those processes are operating (Norenzayan & Heine,

2005). For example, positive self-regard can also be considered

in terms of strivings to be the kind of person viewed as appro-

priate, good, and significant in one’s culture (e.g., Crocker &

Park, 2004; Heine et al., 1999). At this level of analysis, a need

for positive self-regard is a plausible candidate for a psycho-

logical universal, and we propose that the most compelling evo-

lutionary accounts for this motivation will be targeted at this

level (see Heine, Proulx, & Vohs, 2006). In sum, cultural vari-

ation identified by Stage 1 research programs serves to highlight

the level of analysis at which evolutionary accounts might most

successfully be proposed.

Future Challenges and Opportunities in Stage 1 Research

Over the past 20 years, Stage 1 cross-cultural research has en-

joyed a period of tremendous growth. What might happen over

the next 20 years?With the humility to recognize that predicting

the future is best left to fortune-tellers and investment bankers,

we venture to suggest some directions that we think would prove

to be fruitful.

One striking shortcoming of Stage 1 cultural psychological

research thus far is that the majority of the most influential re-

search has been focused on comparisons of North Americans

and East Asians. Perhaps it is understandable that North

Americans specifically and Westerners more generally have

usually served as the point of comparison in these studies, given

that the majority of psychological theories have emerged from

these samples. However, the focus on comparisons between

these two cultural groups has resulted in a pronounced neglect of

other cultures. We suggest that East Asians have been the pri-

mary target of comparison because recent cultural psychological

research has built upon the ideas of Markus and Kitayama

(1991), who proposed a model that was largely based on a

contrast ofWestern and East Asian cultures. The richness of that

model has allowed for a growing number of theoretical advances

to be built upon this initial foundation, leading to a wide variety

of predictions for specific differences in the ways that East

Asians and North Americans are likely to differ in their psy-

chology (e.g., D. Cohen & Gunz, 2002; Heine et al., 1999;

Nisbett et al., 2001; Norenzayan et al., 2002; Oishi et al., 2004;

Suh, 2002). Furthermore, in many ways, cross-cultural com-

parisons between East Asians and North Americans have made

for strong arguments for specific cultural differences, as the

samples that are typically contrasted (university students in the

two cultural regions) are similar in so many other respects (i.e.,

they tend to be from highly industrialized, middle-class, urban

environments, and the participants tend to be highly educated

and cosmopolitan) that there are fewer possible demographic

and cultural variables that could potentially account for the

differences than there are with many other kinds of cross-cul-

tural comparisons. The emergence of reliable and pronounced

differences between groups that share so many important soci-

etal and ecological features suggests that there should be at least

as broad and expansive differences in other regions of the world

that are less industrialized.

We call for cultural psychological research to grow beyond

comparisons of East Asians and Westerners. At present, despite

the growth of cross-cultural research, psychologists still know

embarrassingly little about the psychological processes of the

majority of cultures of the world. There appear to be many

opportunities to identify important cultural differences for

researchers enterprising enough to launch psychological expe-

ditions into relatively unexplored terrain. In particular, we think

the role of culture in psychological functioning should become

especially evident when small-scale societies are studied.

Although such research is methodologically challenging, it

stands to greatly advance understanding of the ways that culture

is implicated in psychology, given the multitude of theoretically

important differences in cultural experience. There has already

been much excellent and influential work conducted with such

groups (e.g., Atran et al., 2005; Bailenson, Shum, Atran, Medin,

& Coley, 2002; Cole, Gay, & Glick, 1968; Gordon, 2004; Hen-

rich et al., 2005; Levinson, 1997; Medin & Atran, 2004; Segall,

Campbell, & Herskovits, 1963), much of it having been done to

make arguments for psychological universals (e.g., Barrett &

Behne, 2005; Ekman, Sorenson, & Friesen, 1969; Levenson,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Lessons Learned from a Lifetime of Applied Social Psychology Research
Ross, Abraham S
Canadian Psychology; Feb 2004; 45, 1; ProQuest Central
pg. 1

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.